Introduction

Introduction.

Vedic Sanskrit provides strong evidence for the phenomenon of ambisyllabicity(1), the idea that the consonantal interlude between two vowels can belong simultaneously to both of the adjacent syllables. Despite several studies purporting to demonstrate ambisyllabicity in modern Germanic languages, including Kahn's classical description of consonantal allophony in English (1976), the existence of ambisyllabicity has been seriously questioned. In this paper I shall support the existence of ambisyllabicity, arguing that in the language of the Rigveda, the oldest extensive literature in Sanskrit, the first consonant of an intervocalic onset cluster is ambisyllabic, at least after a short vowel. Since accent is not a conditioning factor, this is a rather different environment from the stress-sensitive phenomena described in Germanic, and so the data are not susceptible to Kiparsky's (1979) foot-based reanalysis. It will also be seen that the Sanskrit data are not easily tractable by Selkirk's (1982) resyllabification-based alternative to ambisyllabicity, either. Among those who support ambisyllabicity, the details of its representation are a matter of some debate. Borowsky et al. (1984) conjectured that there is no distinction between geminate and ambisyllabic consonants: it is a matter of a language's phonetic implementation to determine whether consonants shared by adjacent syllables will be pronounced as short (ambisyllabically, as in English) or as long (as geminates, as in Italian). On the other hand, Kang (1991) argued that the two phenomena contrast in Korean, and so the two must have different structures. It turns out that Vedic Sanskrit has both ambisyllabicity and long consonants, but they are in complementary distribution. This argues for Borowsky et al.'s conjecture, although with a more fine-grained phonetic implementation than a single language-wide setting.

[Forward to Background: The reality of ambisyllabicity.]

[Up to table of contents]